Tikunakids
/ Crianças Tikuna
Aldeia
Filadelfia
BenjaminConstant,
Amazonas, Brasil
photo
(c)
Marcelo Lima
About
us /
Sobre
a Revista
Apoio
/ sponsors / partners
Presentation
/
Apresentação
Editorial
policy /
Normas
editoriais
Book
Reviews /
Resenhas
Review
Essays /
Ensaios
bibliográficos
Notas
de Leitura
Reading
Notes
Books
received /
Livros
recebidos
Web
Reviews / Links
general
index
table
of contents
indice
geral
Sign
our /
Assine
o nosso
Guest
Book /
Livro
de Visitas
Leia
o nosso /
Read
our
Livro
de Visitas /
Guest
Book
READER'S
FORUM
ESPAÇO
DO LEITOR
|
VOLUME 1
Number 3,
June, 2002
Número
3, Junho, 2002
Cultural Psychology:Theory &
Method - Introduction
Carl Ratner
After many decades of self-imposed
insulation from cultural issues, psychologists
belatedly and reluctantly have been
forced to recognize that
psychological phenomena are constituted
in certain ways by cultural
processes. This recognition has
led to the development of a field of studies
called cultural psychology. Broadly
conceived, it seeks to comprehend
the ways in which psychological
phenomena are part of cultural life and
are interdependent with other cultural
phenomena. In 1910 Dewey wrote
a statement that expresses a central
tenet of cultural psychology. He said
that the processes that animate
and form consciousness lie outside it in
social life. Therefore, the objective
for psychologists is to use mental phenomena
(e.g., perception, emotions) as
clues for comprehending the life
processes that they represent. This
task resembles the paleontologist's
who finds a number and variety of
footprints. From these he goes to work
to construct the structure and the
life habits of the animals that made
them. Just as the paleontologist
would be remiss to restrict his attention to
describing footprints themselves,
analyzing their elements, comparing
them to each other, and discovering
the laws of their arrangement in
space—while failing to explore the
living organisms and habits that they
represent—so psychologists are remiss
in restricting their attention to
describing states of consciousness,
their elements, and interactions, while
failing to link consciousness to
real-life processes of human beings. "The
supposition that these states [of
consciousness] are somehow existent by
themselves and in this existence
provide the psychologist with readymade
material is just the supreme case
of the 'psychological fallacy'"
(Dewey, 1910, p. 250; see also Vygotsky,
1997a, pp. 272–273, 327 for a
remarkably similar statement).
Of course, psychological phenomena
have a more complex relationship
to social life than footprints have
to forces that form them. However,
Dewey is metaphor correctly emphasizes
the social formation of the
psyche.
Studying cultural processes in order
to understand psychology is a
daunting task. It requires a specific,
comprehensive conceptualization of
what culture is and how it encompasses
human psychology (see Ratner,
2000a). Progress in this area is
difficult for several reasons. The structures of
modern societies militate against
scientifically understanding psychology
as a cultural phenomenon. Social
life in contemporary societies appears to
be a myriad of diverse factors that
have no ostensible relationship to each
other. Furthermore, each individual
occupies diverse social positions, is
exposed to diverse information,
and has substantial freedom of choice.
Given this dizzying diversity, how
can there be anything coherent we can
call "culture"? And how can a dizzying
diversity of cultural factors be
related to human psychology in any
meaningful way, especially when
human psychology appears to be dizzyingly
diverse? Individuals seem to
differ enormously in their perceptions,
emotions, reasoning, memory, and
intelligence. If culture and psychology
are indefinite, incoherent, and ineffable
then no systematic relationship
could exist between them.
Difficulties in relating psychology
to culture are compounded by
the institutional specialization
of psychology as an academic discipline.
Segregated from social sciences
such as sociology, anthropology, history,
and political science, psychology
appears to be unrelated to the social
issues that are covered by these
disciplines.
An additional factor that obscures
the cultural nature of psychology
is Western ideology. It tends to
regard human psychology as an individual
or universal phenomenon, equally
unrelated to social factors.
These obstacles have impeded the
development of cultural psychology
(see Ratner, 1993a, 1997b, 1999
for examples). Most cultural psychologists
manifest little interest in formulating
a specific and thorough conception
of culture to guide their work.
They often satisfy themselves with casual,
incomplete, and implicit notions
such as "Psychological processes take
place in cultural settings," "Psychological
processes are socially constructed
and shared," "Psychological processes
are mediated by cultural tools"
(Wertsch), and "Cultural psychology
is the study of intentional worlds"
(Shweder). Cultural psychologists
rarely consult sociological or historical
research concerning the formation,
maintenance, and change of social
organizations, classes, conditions,
norms, and systems. Most cultural psychologists
express the belief that they are
abandoning psychology if they
systematically study culture. Many
anthropologists and psychologists
tend to regard culture as a platform
on which psychology stands. The
platform model acknowledges that
people in New Guinea and Germany
behave, reason, remember, feel,
and perceive in different ways. It tells us
that these phenomena are cultural;
however, it does not tell us how they
are cultural—that is, what they
have to do with the particular societies in
which they occur. The very connection
between culture and psychology is
suspended although psychology is
recognized as occurring in (or on)
culture. This kind of research requires
no familiarity with social systems
because it describes psychological
phenomena per se.
Even research from the positivistic
standpoint, which attempts to
correlate specific cultural factors
with psychological phenomena, generally
overlooks the internal relationship,
between them. It discovers that
commercial activity, for example,
is associated with depression. However,
it does not disclose how depression
recapitulates commerce, or how the
characteristics of depression reflect
commerce. Thus, the cultural character
of depression is obscured even though
its correlation with a cultural
factor has been identified.
Lacking a specific and thorough
conception of culture has led to
eclecticism in cultural psychology.
Virtually any topic, theoretical
viewpoint, and methodology are accepted
within the rubric of cultural
psychology. There is little integration
of hypotheses or findings. In
addition, many crucial components
of culture are overlooked or discounted.
The result is little agreement or
understanding about the cultural
aspects of psychology—or what it
means to say that psychology is
cultural.
The disinterest in culture reaches
its apex in postmodernist notions of
culture, agency, and psychology.
Postmodernism renounces culture as a
set of shared psychological phenomena
that are grounded in organized
social life. Postmodernists construe
society as inchoate, indefinite, and
incapable of providing a structure
to psychological phenomena. They also
construe society as depersonalized
and deleterious so that individuals
must find fulfillment in personal
acts apart from social influence. In this
view, individuals must form their
own personal psychologies on the basis
of unique needs and choices. This
antisocial view of psychology has
squelched the promise of cultural
psychology to deepen our understanding
of cultural aspects of psychology.
To be worthy of its name, cultural
psychology must penetrate
beneath apparent fragmentation,
incoherence, and disorder to discover
regularities and relationships.
This, after all, is the task of all science. Just
as natural science has discovered
parsimonious principles and laws
that integrally explain an enormous
diversity of seemingly disparate
phenomena—the falling of an apple
and the revolving of planets are all
forms of gravity—so social science
can discover that culture is an organized,
coherent system; psychological phenomena
are socially shared and
distributed; and psychological phenomena
have definite social origins,
characteristics, and functions.
As Hegel said, the real is rational.
Developing the science of cultural
psychology requires a renewed
commitment to deeply understand
culture both in general terms ("What
is human culture?") and in specific
terms ("What is Polish culture?"). A
concentrated analysis of culture
is necessary to provide a conceptual basis
to cultural psychology that will
explain the cultural origins, formation,
and function of psychological phenomena.
An analysis of culture will also
elucidate the active role of human
agency within social life.
In the first part of Cultural
Psychology:Theory & Method
(N.Y.: Plenum, 2002), I articulate
a specific and comprehensive
conception of culture. I identify
its crucial components, the manner in
which they are integrated together,
and the role of psychological
phenomena within them. My theoretical
perspective draws on Vygotsky's
activity theory. I seek to develop
its philosophical and political underpinnings
in new ways that can guide cultural
psychology to become
a scientific discipline with practical
importance.
In Part 2, I develop methodological
procedures for testing the theoretical
issues raised in Part 1. I explain
the principles of a methodology
that can elucidate the distintive
and complex subject matter of cultural
psychology. I explain how interview
techniques can be refined to probe
for cultural themes in psychological
phenomena. I also explain how narrative
statements can be analyzed to elucidate
cultural themes. The final
chapter designs an empirical investigation
into the cultural psychology of
moral reasoning in children. It
conceptualizes cultural aspects of moral
reasoning which are typically overlooked
in structuralist accounts such as
Kohlberg's. It then describes a
procedure for identifying cultural aspects
of moral reasoning. This project
brings together theoretical and empirical
issues that have been discussed
in earlier chapters.
The theoretical and methodological
approach I outline is not meant
to apply to every aspect of human
psychology. My approach is confined
to describing and explaining the
specific cultural content that is embedded
in psychological phenomena shared
by members of a particular
society (or subsociety). In other
words, I seek to describe and explain the
characteristics of psychological
phenomena that originate in, are formed
by, and function to promulgate particular
cultural activities, artifacts, and
concepts that comprise a definite
social system. Other aspects of psychology
are comprised of idiosyncratic features
that originate in unique experiences
and biological processes of an individual.
For example, a boy who
is raised by a soft-spoken mother
may grow up to be soft-spoken. His
manner is more a function of his
idiosyncratic experience than of prevalent
social activities. Or a girl whose
father committed suicide in her presence
becomes insecure and paranoid. These
psychological phenomena are
the province of clinical psychology,
not cultural psychology.
In addition, there are general aspects
of human psychology that are
common to all people. These psychological
universals include the fact
that all humans engage in self-reflection
and volitional action, are profoundly
open to experience and learning,
understand the nature of things
and use this understanding to solve
problems intelligently, communicate
via language, think and remember
in symbolic terms, organize emotions
and perceptions around cognitive
interpretations, and engage in logical
reasoning. These universal, general
aspects of human psychology are
devoid of content and they do not
reflect particular social activities, artifacts,
or concepts. They evidently depend
on some social experience, as its
absence impedes the development
of these general psychological features
(Ratner, 1991, pp. 11–68). However,
they seem to reflect general aspects of
social experience rather than specific
characteristics. For instance,
parental regulation of behavior
may be necessary to foster self-control,
volition, planning, and self-consciousness.
The general occurrence of parents
interrupting, guiding, and encouraging
behavior fosters these in all
children, regardless of the specific
manner in which parents exercise this
regulation. The specific manner
in which regulation is exercised—for
example, by verbal threats, physical
constraint, patient explanation, tolerance
for continued impulsiveness on the
part of the child—would affect
the specific content of the child's
self-concept, volition, and emotional
expression (Ratner, 1991, pp. 113–198).
The cultural psychology that I outline
in Cultural Psychology:Theory & Method
is confined to describing and explaining
specific, content-laden characteristics of psychology. I concentrate on
this domain because it has tremendous (but
undeveloped) potential for enhancing
our understanding of human
psychology. It also contains the
greatest potential for practically improving
psychological functioning. We can
alter cultural factors that affect
the psychology of many people, whereas
general aspects of psychology
(e.g., emotions depend on cognitive
appraisals) cannot be altered, and
individual aspects can be altered
only in piecemeal fashion, and even this
is difficult as long as social activities,
artifacts, and concepts continue to
foster them.
REFERENCES
Dewey, J. (1910). The influence
of Darwin on philosophy and other essays on contemporary thought. New York:
Holt.
Ratner, C. (1991). Vygotsky’s
sociohistorical psychology and its contemporary applications. New york:
Plenum.
Ratner, C. (1993a). Review
of D’Andrade and Strauss, Human motives and cultural models. Journal of
Mind and Behavior, 14, 89–94.
Ratner, C. (1997b). In defense
of activity theory. Culture and Psychology, 3, 211–223.
Ratner, C. (1999). Three
approaches to cultural psychology: A critique. Cultural Dynamics, 11, 7–31.
Ratner, C. (2000a). Outline
of a coherent, comprehensive concept of culture. Cross-Cultural Psychology
Bulletin, 34(1 & 2), 5–11.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1997a).
Educational psychology. Boca Raton, FL: St. Lucie Press. (Originally written
1921)
copyright
(c) 2002 Centro de Estudos e Pesquisas Armando de Oliveira
Souza CEPAOS
return
to CEPAOS Review Homepage
endereço
mailing
address:
Caixa
Postal 12833
São
Paulo, 04009-970,
Brasil
phone
+ fax: (55) 11 - 50837182 Brasil
fax:
303-200-8866
USA
www.cepaos.cjb.net
www.cepaosreview.cjb.net |